top of page

FLUENCY INSTRUCTION, ANOTHER COMBATANT IN THE READING WARS, OR AN INNOCENT BYSTANDER?

Fluency is seen as one of the central tendencies of reading instruction. It can be defined as the ability to read quickly and with ease. However, within the academic discourse of education instruction, fluency is looked at both as a skill and as a type of instruction. Fluency instruction, referring to instruction specifically meant to increase a student’s ability to read fluently. However,  within the evidence-based education community there remains some skepticism of fluency instruction. In many ways, fluent reading is less the representation of one or two skills, but more the successful marker of a nearly proficient reader. As proficient reading requires students to be past the decoding stage of learning to read and to know most words on sight. To put it more plainly, to read fluently, is to know how to read and by this logic, all reading instruction could hypothetically be viewed as fluency instruction. However, within the literature fluency instruction usually refers to teaching methodologies, specifically meant to increase the speed of which students recognize sight words. 


I believe fluency methodologies of instruction have been criticized for several reasons. Firstly, a large part of the evidence-based reading community has formed around the rallying cry of phonetic instruction and some phonics enthusiasts see fluency instruction as a competing ideology of literacy instruction. Secondly, many fluency instructional strategies appear to be hyper-specific, and albeit strangely focused on timing how fast students can read. Lastly, many critics have perhaps rightfully so pointed out that fluency is likely a by-product of multiple other skills, and not necessarily a wholly unique skill unto itself. 

Because of these reasons, I have to admit, I myself have been quite skeptical of fluency instruction. However, I recently reformed my opinion, after realizing that there are several quite notable experts including Dr. Tim Shanahan and Dr. John Hattie, who have pointed out that fluency specific instruction, is a high yield strategy according to meta-analysis. According to Hattie’s meta-study, Repeated Reading, (which is a specific fluency intervention)  has an ES of .75, making it a higher yield strategy for literacy instruction than phonics instruction, in general, according to Dr. Hattie’s work. Dr. Shanahan who chaired one of the largest language instruction meta-studies ever done wrote “I chaired the NRP sub-panel that reviewed the research on fluency teaching, and our summary of that research concluded that such teaching was beneficial to their reading development by a wide range of measures. Kids who received fluency instruction simply read better than those who did not. These outcomes were obtained with children in regular classrooms (Grades 1-4) and with struggling readers (Grades 1-12).” (Shanahan, 2020)” 


That being said The National Reading Panel (NICHHD) meta-analysis on the use of Repeated Reading for increasing fluency found a mean ES of .41, making repeated reading a decidedly moderately effective reading intervention. (NICHHD, 2001). Since then there have been three studies comparing Multiple Exemplars to Repeated Reading, one in 2001 by Eckert et al, 2007 by Ardoin et al, and one in 2007 by McCurdy et al. Each one of these studies showed a very minimal advantage to repeated reading interventions. In 2019 Zimmerman et al, conducted a meta-study for the Iowa Reading Research Centre comparing repetitive fluency interventions vs nonrepetitive fluency interventions. Their study showed a very slight benefit for nonrepetitive interventions; however, the ES, was so low that they did not even publish, the actual ES and just concluded that the ES was lower than a standard deviation of .2 In other words, their results were so insignificant, they didn’t even fully publish them.


Personally, I think this is more of an issue of when to use this intervention, rather than a matter of if to use this intervention. As Dr. Noam Chomsky, Linea Ehri, and Tim Shanahan have all pointed out “fluency work is likely most helpful at what Linnea Ehri has described as the consolidation or automaticity stages of decoding development” (Shanahan, 2020). If we put aside the academic discourse, and the quantitative data for a moment, there is a certain logic, to using different types of instruction more or less at different stages. For example, when students are first learning how to read, ultimately, the first things, we need them to learn are the alphabet, the core sounds of the alphabet, and that these sounds exist as the building blocks of words. Next, we want students to be able to use phonetic principles to decode words. However, once students are fluent at decoding, we want them to build their familiarity with their language to the point that reading is automatic. This is why I created what I call the Reading instruction continuum. 

Fluency Instruction IMG001.png

This continuum lays out how much instruction should be phonetic, comprehension, or fluency based, at each stage of reading. While these stages of reading are supposed to correspond with grades, not all students read at their grade level. That being said, Pre-Reading as I see it, is pre-kindergarten to grade 1, Novice reading is grade 2-3, Decoding Reading is grades 4-6, and Fluent Reading is grades 7 and up. One final note on this topic, while the continuum of reading is my concept, the labels I have used for reading stages is not.

This all being said, there are multiple types of fluency instruction that exist. Repeated Reading is the most commonly used intervention. Repeated reading involves reading the same text over and over again, to increase fluency. Multiple Exemplars works on the same principle as Repeated Reading; except, while the words stay the same, the actual piece of writing changes, after each reading attempt. Timed Reading works very similarly to Multiple Exemplars, except that students are given different pieces of reading material, all at a specific grade level, but without the use of specific words. In Timed Reading, the student has to keep practice reading at a specific level, until they reach a set speed of reading at 100 words per minute. All of these strategies can be essentially divided into two categories, repetitive fluency interventions vs nonrepetitive fluency interventions. 


As I alluded to earlier, Repeated Reading is the most evidence-based of these strategies. However, I do not personally feel beholden to any of these strategies, as, in my personal opinion, they do come across as quite unengaging for students. Personally, I like to have students read a text out loud, while an adult listens and reads with them to help decode, say, and explain difficult words. This can be done, with large groups, small groups, individuals, and at home with parents. This strategy also works great for cross-curricular lessons, I love to use it for history class. However, despite looking, I have not found any significant data on the efficacy of this strategy. 


Written by Nathaniel Hansford

Last Edited: 12/21/2020


References:

Rosenshine, Barak, and Carla Meister. “Reciprocal Teaching: A Review of the Research.” Review of Educational Research, vol. 64, no. 4, 1994, pp. 479–530. JSTOR, www.jstor.org/stable/1170585. Accessed 6 May 2020.


Finders, Margaret, and Phyllis Balcerzak. “It's Time to Revise K-W-L.” Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, vol. 56, no. 6, 2013, pp. 460–460. JSTOR, www.jstor.org/stable/41827878. Accessed 6 May 2020.


Zimmerman et al. (2019). Research Brief: Meta-Analysis Shows Non-Repetitive Reading Fluency Interventions as Options for Teachers. Iowa Reading Research Centre. Retrieved from <https://iowareadingresearch.org/blog/research-brief-non-repetitive-reading-fluency-intervention>. 


Pruitt, Beth Anne, and Justin T. Cooper. “Ready, Set, Go: Three Strategies to Build Reading Fluency.” Beyond Behavior, vol. 17, no. 3, 2008, pp. 8–13. JSTOR, www.jstor.org/stable/24011933. Accessed 6 May 2020. 


Ardoin, Scott P., et al. “Promoting Generalization of Reading: A Comparison of Two Fluency-Based Interventions for Improving General Education Student's Oral Reading Rate.” Journal of Behavioral Education, vol. 17, no. 3, 2008, pp. 237–252. JSTOR, www.jstor.org/stable/41824437. Accessed 6 May 2020.


McCurdy, M., Daly, E., Gortmaker, V., Bonfiglio, C., <fe Persampieri, M. (2007). Use of brief instructional trials to identify small group reading strategies: A two experiment study. Journal of

Behavioral Education, 76(1), 7-26. 


Eckert, T. L., Ardoin, S. P., Daly, E. J., III, «fe Martens, B. K. (2002). Improving oral reading fluency: A  brief experimental analysis of combining an antecedent intervention with consequences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 271.


Ardoin, S. P., McCall, M., & Klubnik, C. (2007). Promoting generalization of oral reading fluency:

Providing drill versus practice opportunities. Journal of Behavioral Education, 7(5(1),


J, Hattie. (2017). Hattie Ranking: 252 Influences And Effect Sizes Related To Student Achievement. Corwin. Retrieved from <https://visible-learning.org/hattie-ranking-influences-effect-sizes-learning-achievement/>.


T, Shanahan. (2019). When should reading instruction begin? Reading Rockets. Retrieved from <https://www.readingrockets.org/blogs/shanahan-literacy/when-should-reading-instruction-begin>. 


T, Shanahan. (2019). Wake Up Reading Wars Combatants: Fluency Instruction is Part of the Science of Reading. Reading Rockets. Retrieved from <https://www.readingrockets.org/blogs/shanahan-literacy/wake-reading-wars-combatants-fluency-instruction-part-science-reading>. 


Copyright © 2018 Pedagogy Non Grata  - All Rights Reserved.

bottom of page